How, then, could the strabismus have produced such a radical change in cortical function? To answer this we need to consider how the two eyes normally act together. What the strabismus had changed was the relationship between the stimuli to the two eyes. When we look at a scene, the images in the two retinas from any point in the scene normally fall on locations that are the same distance and in the same direction from the two foveas--they fall on corresponding points. If a binocular cell in the cortex happens to be activated when an image falls on the left retina--if the cell's receptive field is crossed by a dark-light contour whose orientation is exactly right for the cell--then that cell will also be excited by the image on the right retina, for three reasons: (1) the images fall on the same parts of the two retinas, (2) a binocular cell (unless it is specialized for depth) has its receptive fields in exactly the same parts of the two retinas, and (3) the orientation preferences of binocular cells are always the same in the two eyes. If the eyes are not parallel, reason 1 obviously no longer applies: with the images no longer in concordance, if at a given moment a cell happens to be told to fire by one eye, whether the other eye will also be telling the cell to fire is a matter of chance. This, as far as a single cell is concerned, would seem to be the only thing that changes in strabismus. Somehow, in a young kitten, the perpetuation over weeks or months of this state of affairs, in which the signals from the two eyes are no longer concordant, causes the weaker of the two sets of connections to the cell to weaken even further and often for practical purposes to disappear. Thus we have an example of ill effects coming not as a result of removing or withholding a stimulus, but merely as a result of disrupting the normal time relationships between two sets of stimuli--a subtle insult indeed, considering the gravity of the consequences. In these experiments, monkeys gave the same results as kittens; it therefore seems likely that strabismus leads to the same consequences in humans. Clinically, in someone with a long-standing alternating strabismus, even if the strabismus is repaired, the person does not usually regain the ability to see depth. The surgeon can bring the two eyes into alignment only to the nearest few degrees. Perhaps the failure to recover is due to the loss of the person's ability to make up the residual deficit, to fuse the two images perfectly by bringing the eyes into alignment to the nearest few minutes of arc. Surgically repairing the strabismus aligns the eyes well enough so that in a normal person the neural mechanisms would be sufficient to take care of the remaining few degrees of fine adjustment, but in a strabismic person these are the very mechanisms, including binocular cells in the cortex, that have been disrupted. To get recovery would presumably require protracted reestablishment of perfect alignment in the two eyes, something that requires normal muscle alignment plus an alignment depending on binocular vision.